Limitations on Comparative Dietary Proofs |
Editorial note: In the following, the term "comparative proofs" refers to the comparative proofs for particular diets. It is not a general reference to any/all comparative studies.
Logical and Structural Limitations |
First, let's modify the "proof" in Fit Food for Humanity as follows. Based on the more recent knowledge now available, replace the collective category for vegetarian apes with two more-
Next, note that Mills (The Comparative Anatomy of Eating) provides a lengthy "proof" that purports to show humans are herbivores. Finally, note that Voegtlin [1975] as cited in Fallon and Enig [1997], provides a comparative anatomy analysis that indicates humans are actually natural carnivores. See Functional and Structural Comparison of Man's Digestive Tract with that of a Dog or Sheep (offsite) for a comparative anatomy analysis from Voegtlin [1975].
Four "proofs"--but what do they prove? Thus, as a result of producing and collecting the "proofs" cited above, we will have 4 different types of comparative "proofs":
It should be mentioned here that some who present comparative "proofs" openly admit their subjective nature (though the point is often not given proper emphasis, for the obvious reasons), whereas others are not so candid.
Additionally, one other point bears repeating here. Let us pretend that comparison of a list of features somehow constitutes hard "proof" for a specific diet. It then follows that one must ask relevant questions, listed below:
By ignoring or rationalizing away the differences, the comparative "proofs" of diet render themselves logically invalid. In a discussion of the topic of comparative studies of adaptation, Ridley [1983,
The very first requirement, then, for a test of a comparative hypothesis is that the criterion for including a species in the test is independent of whether it supports the hypothesis. This criterion may seem obvious; it may seem that it does not require statement. But it is enough to rule out the entire [comparative] literature (with one exception) from before about 1960. That exception is the French biologist Etienne Rabaud.
The "Expensive Tissue Hypothesis" [Aiello and Wheeler 1995] and related research discussed in the previous section suggest that the evolution of our brain and higher human intelligence is in part the result of a diet that included significant amounts of animal foods. The energy budget and life history events of humans are unique, relevant, and should be covered in a legitimate comparative study. The comparative "proofs" ignore
Similar functions can be served by dissimilar forms. John McArdle, Ph.D., a scientific advisor to the American Anti-
This describes the approach of Anapol and Lee [1994], whose research analyzed the morphology of the jaws and teeth of 8 species of platyrrhine monkeys from the Surinam rainforest. Their research found statistically significant differences that were associated with diet in some of the measures of tooth, jaw, and masticatory musculature.
Examination of the Anapol and Lee [1994] research versus the comparative "proofs" of diet illustrates some important points:
As the form/
GO TO NEXT PART OF ARTICLE
Return to beginning of article
Back to Research-Based Appraisals of Alternative Diet Lore
Then, we simply note that the above cannot all be true as they are contradictory. This points to the conclusion that comparative "proofs" of diet are subjective and indeterminate--
There is no logical validation of the list comparison process.
That is, there is no logical validation for the implicit assumption that comparing two or more species via a list of features constitutes proof that a particular diet is natural or optimal. Comparative studies can be powerful tools for analysis, but there is so much variation in nature that one cannot logically derive a "should" (i.e., you should eat this diet) from an "is" derived from other species' diets (i.e., that other, allegedly similar species, eat the same type of diet). The presence of substantial morphological similarity between two different species does not prove, or even imply, that their diets must be similar. The idea that in nature a "should" can be derived from an "is" (of another species) in such a fashion is a major logical fallacy of the comparative "proofs" for particular diets.
For the obvious reasons (they are too heavily subjective), the above questions are unanswered. The advocates of comparative "proofs" are assuming that a matching list somehow "proves" their claims. In reality, it proves nothing--
The comparative method is used to look for associations (between the characters of organisms) which natural selection, or some other causal principle, might be expected to give rise to. It can test hypotheses about the cause of adaptive patterns. The result of this test is an association or correlation. The mere association of two characters, A and B, does not tell us whether in nature A was the cause of B,
The comparative "proofs" focus only on similarities while ignoring differences.
In legitimate applications of comparative anatomy and/or physiology, the differences--
The most rudimentary kinds of comparative study are those which, after looking through the literature more or less thoroughly, present a list of only the species (or some of them) which conform to the hypothesis... Such a list may usefully demonstrate that a trait is taxonomically widespread. Or it may suggest a hypothesis. But it cannot test one. If the method does not also list the species that do not conform to the hypothesis it is analogous to selecting only the supporting part of the results from a mass of experimental data...
The basic problem of studying similarities while deliberately ignoring differences is that it constitutes picking-
The comparative "proofs" ignore the fossil record and evolution.
The comparative "proofs" are dated and do not reflect current knowledge of
The comparative "proofs" assume dietary categories are discrete (distinct), while in nature diets form a continuum.
The above points were covered in previous sections.
The comparative "proofs" ignore the important features that make humans unique
In particular, these features are generally ignored:
As discussed in the previous section, the topic of evolution of the brain vs. diet is usually not covered. Our brain allows us to develop technology (e.g., tools and cooking), which can impact our morphology via the behavior/
Comparative "proofs" assume the form/
Here the word "necessary" is used in its logical sense; i.e., that function necessarily follows form, and that the form/
It is also important to remember that the relation between form (anatomy/
The form/function relationship can be dissimilar even in closely related species. Sussman [1987] comments that
When comparing species-
Overall systemic constraints as important as individual functions/
Function cannot be stressed more than form. Their separation from each other, and their individual isolation from the intact working organism, are both essential, but to a large extent necessarily incomplete, operations. The same is true of the abstraction of biochemical facts. Form, chemistry, and function are indissolubly united, and it may truly be said that from the taxonomic standpoint all characters of the body, whatever their nature, have a fundamental equivalency... To discuss them on the basis of
Research on the form/
Some examples of research studies that tried to document a form/
Thus we see that a narrowly focused study, within a closely related group of species, might find possible evidence of a form/
Rabaud was concerned with Cuverian laws of the relationship between anatomy and niche, such as that herbivores have flat, grinding teeth and carnivores sharp, piercing teeth. Rabaud first looked systematically at these laws in his book of 1911, and later in a longer work on Les phenomenes de convergence en biologie (1925). In 1925 he summarized, systematically, such standard examples of [evolutionary] convergence as adaptations for flying and swimming. Each systematic summary led to the same conclusion: "one can recognize no necessary relation between the form and the way of life of the organisms considered" (1925,
The authors attempt to substantiate the correlation between diet and gastrointestinal morphology among a series of Primates; they conclude that the distinctions are not so obvious as in other orders and in most cases do not permit reliable conclusions to be drawn, while in some cases the evidence is contradictory.
The above reflects the reality that analysis of the gut is not so simplistic as one might expect from the comparative "proofs." (In later sections, more recent studies--
The results of this study suggest that there is no definitive or straightforward relationship between dietary type and the functional morphology of the masticatory apparatus discernible among the taxa studied, although some encouraging patterns may be observed. Primate dietary behavior is flexible and may not be predicted with certainty on morphological grounds.
Once again, efforts were unsuccessful in establishing a strong linkage between form and function in the context of diet and morphology.
The work of Anapol and Lee is a good example of a legitimate application of comparative anatomy; in contrast, the comparative proofs of diet are really misapplications of comparative anatomy.
(Limitations on Comparative Dietary Proofs, cont., plus Counterexamples)
SEE TABLE OF CONTENTS FOR:
PART 1 PART 2 PART 3 PART 4 PART 5 PART 6 PART 7 PART 8 PART 9
GO TO PART 1 - Brief Overview: What is the Relevance of Comparative Anatomical and Physiological "Proofs"?
GO TO PART 2 - Looking at Ape Diets: Myths, Realities, and Rationalizations
GO TO PART 3 - The Fossil-Record Evidence about
GO TO PART 4 - Intelligence, Evolution of the Human Brain,
GO TO PART 5 - Limitations on Comparative Dietary Proofs
GO TO PART 6 - What Comparative Anatomy Does and Doesn't Tell Us about
GO TO PART 7 - Insights about Human Nutrition & Digestion from Comparative Physiology
GO TO PART 8 - Further Issues in the Debate over Omnivorous vs. Vegetarian Diets
GO TO PART 9 - Conclusions: The End, or The Beginning of a New Approach to